single-jc.php

JACIII Vol.28 No.3 pp. 502-510
doi: 10.20965/jaciii.2024.p0502
(2024)

Research Paper:

Effect of Different Measurement Tasks on the Frequency of Microsaccades

Toumi Ohara and Fumiya Kinoshita ORCID Icon

Electrical and Computer Engineering, Graduate School of Engineering, Toyama Prefectural University
5180 Kurokawa, Imizu, Toyama 939-0398, Japan

Corresponding author

Received:
September 28, 2023
Accepted:
November 23, 2023
Published:
May 20, 2024
Keywords:
covert attention, fixational eye movement, microsaccades, interval of occurrence, gaze point
Abstract

In recent years, several attempts have been made to quantitatively evaluate covert attention using microsaccades. However, several unclear aspects exist regarding the measurement method of microsaccades, and a unified analysis method does not exist. Therefore, the current status is such that the interpretation of the results is divided among the research groups. To address this problem, empirical studies on microsaccades must be accumulated and measured and evaluated using a unified method. Therefore, in this study, to accumulate empirical studies on microsaccades, an experiment was conducted to investigate the effect of the presence or absence of gazing at a fixation point on the interval of occurrence of microsaccades in a measurement task. The participants were 15 healthy young people, and we compared the following two types of measurement tasks. Task-I: The participants freely visually searched a white wall 1 m away for 120 s. Task-II: The participants gazed at a fixation point located 1 m ahead at eye level for 120 s. For the microsaccade detection algorithm, we adopted a method imitating the EK method proposed by Engbert and Kliegl in 2003, divided 120 s of time-series data and analyzed it every 2 s, and subsequently evaluated the time-series data for the entire 120 s by concatenating them. Consequently, the interval of occurrence of microsaccades during Task-II decreased by more than 1 Hz compared with that of Task-I (p<0.05). The study confirmed that the presence or absence of gazing at the fixation point during microsaccade measurement affected the interval of occurrence of microsaccades.

Eye movement over 2 s (velocity coordinate)

Eye movement over 2 s (velocity coordinate)

Cite this article as:
T. Ohara and F. Kinoshita, “Effect of Different Measurement Tasks on the Frequency of Microsaccades,” J. Adv. Comput. Intell. Intell. Inform., Vol.28 No.3, pp. 502-510, 2024.
Data files:
References
  1. [1] H. Kaneko, “Fixational eye movements,” The J. of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers, Vol.63, No.11, pp. 1538-1539, 2009 (in Japanese). https://doi.org/10.3169/itej.63.1538
  2. [2] I. P. V. Troxler, “Über das Verschwinden gegebener Gegenstande innerhalb unseres Gesichtskreises,” Ophthalmologische Bibliothek, Vol.2, No.2, pp. 1-53, 1804 (in German).
  3. [3] H. Helmholtz, “Handbuch der physiologischen Optik,” Leopold Voss, 1867 (in German).
  4. [4] T. N. Cornsweet, “Determination of the stimuli for involuntary drifts and saccadic eye movements,” J. of the Optical Society of America, Vol.46, No.11, pp. 987-993, 1956. https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.46.000987
  5. [5] B. L. Zuber, “Models of Oculomotor Behavior and Control,” CRC Press, 1981.
  6. [6] G. Westheimer, “The spatial sense of the eye. proctor lecture,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, Vol.18, No.9, pp. 893-912, 1979.
  7. [7] D. M. Snodderly, I. Kagan, and M. Gur, “Selective activation of visual cortex neurons by fixational eye movements: Implications for neural coding,” Visual Neuroscience, Vol.18, No.2, pp. 259-277, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523801182118
  8. [8] S. Martinez-Conde, S. L. Macknik, and D. H. Hubel, “The function of bursts of spikes during visual fixation in the awake primate lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex,” Proc. of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol.99, No.21, pp. 13920-13925, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.212500599
  9. [9] T. Kohama, K. Shinkai, and S. Usui, “Quantitatively measuring visual attention by analyzing microsaccades,” The J. of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers, Vol.52, No.4, pp. 571-576, 1998 (in Japanese). https://doi.org/10.3169/itej.52.571
  10. [10] Z. M. Hafed and J. J. Clark, “Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts,” Vision Research, Vol.42, No.22, pp. 2533-2545, 2002. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00263-8
  11. [11] R. Engbert and R. Kliegl, “Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention,” Vision Research, Vol.43, No.9, pp. 1035-1045, 2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00084-1
  12. [12] S. Miki and Y. Hirata, “Microsaccades generated during car driving,” 2013 35th Annual Int. Conf. of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), pp. 2148-2151, 2013. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2013.6609959
  13. [13] Z. Kapoula et al., “Distinctive features of mi-crosaccades in Alzheimer’s disease and in mild cognitive impairment,” AGE, Vol.36, No.2, pp. 535-543, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-013-9582-3
  14. [14] M. Takahashi, H. Isogai, and J. L. V. Raalte, “Attempts to detect microsaccades from eye movements in anticipatory response situation: Introducing anticipatory response tasks toward tennis serve,” J. of Japan Society of Sports Industry, Vol.28, No.1, pp. 13-29, 2018 (in Japanese). https://doi.org/10.5997/sposun.28.1_13
  15. [15] S. Endo, T. Kohama, and D. Noguchi, “Continuous inhibition of microsaccades in attentional concentration,” ITE Technical Report, Vol.37, No.12, pp. 51-54, 2013 (in Japanese). https://doi.org/10.11485/itetr.37.12.0_51
  16. [16] J. Otero-Millan et al., “Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccades,” J. of Vision, Vol.14, No.2, Article No.18, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1167/14.2.18
  17. [17] A. Mihali, B. van Opheusden, and W. J. Ma, “Bayesian microsaccade detection,” J. of Vision, Vol.17, No.1, Article No.13, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1167/17.1.13
  18. [18] J. Emoto and Y. Hirata, “Realtime microsaccade detection with convolutional neural network,” The IEICE Trans. on Information and Systems (Japanese Edition), Vol.J101-D, No.2, pp. 456-467, 2018 (in Japanese).
  19. [19] T. Ohara and F. Kinoshita, “Effect of alcohol consumption on the frequency of microsaccades,” J. Adv. Comput. Intell. Intell. Inform., Vol.27, No.2, pp. 148-153, 2023. https://doi.org/10.20965/jaciii.2023.p0148
  20. [20] R. Engbert, “Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception,” Progress in Brain Research, Vol.154, Part A, pp. 177-192, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)54009-9
  21. [21] K. Inagaki and S. Usui, “Neuronal mechanisms for generation of microsaccades,” The Brain & Neural Networks, Vol.19, No.3, pp. 135-144, 2012 (in Japanese).
  22. [22] M. I. Posner, “Orienting of attention,” Quarterly J. of Experimental Psychology, Vol.32, No.1, pp. 3-25, 1980. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231

*This site is desgined based on HTML5 and CSS3 for modern browsers, e.g. Chrome, Firefox, Safari, Edge, Opera.

Last updated on Jun. 03, 2024