Paper:
Hazard Perception and Anchoring: A Comparison of the Three Models Explaining the Anchoring Effect
Kazuhisa Nagaya and Kazuya Nakayachi
Doshisha University
1-3 Tatara Miyakodani, Kyotanabe, Kyoto 610-0394, Japan
- [1] C. Keller, M. Siegrist, and H. Gutscher, “The role of the affect and availability heuristics in risk communication,” Risk Anal., Vol.26, pp. 631-639, 2006.
- [2] M. Siegrist and H. Gutscher, “Natural hazards and motivation for mitigation behavior: people cannot predict the affect evoked by a severe flood,” Risk Anal., Vol.28, pp. 771-778, 2008.
- [3] A. Wildavsky, “No risk is the highest risk of all,” American Scientist, Vol.67, pp. 32-37, 1979.
- [4] J. D. Graham and J. B. Wiener, “Risk vs. risk: Tradeoffs in protecting health and the environment,” Harvard University Press, 1995.
- [5] D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, and A. Tversky (Eds.), “Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases,” Cambridge University Press, 1982.
- [6] A. Tversky, and D. Kahneman, “Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability,” In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), “Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases,” Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 163-189, 1982.
- [7] C. MacLeod and L. Campbell, “Memory accessibility and probability judgments: An experimental evaluation of the availability heuristic,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.63, pp. 890-902, 1992.
- [8] D. Kahneman and A. Tversky, “Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness,” Cognitive Psychology, Vol.3, pp. 430-454, 1972.
- [9] A. Tversky and D. Kahneman, “Belief in the law of small numbers,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol.76, pp. 105-110, 1971.
- [10] M. L. Finucane, A. Alhakami, P. Slovic, and S. M. Johnson, “The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits,” J. of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol.13, pp. 1-17, 2000.
- [11] P. Slovic, M. Finucane, E. Peters, and D. G. MacGregor, “The affect heuristic,” In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman, (Eds.), “Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment,” Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 397-420, 2002.
- [12] A. Tversky and D. Kahneman, “Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases,” Science, Vol.185, pp. 1124-1131, 1974.
- [13] A. Furnham and H. C. Boo, “A literature review of the anchoring effect,” J. of Socio-Economics, Vol.40, pp. 35-42, 2011.
- [14] S. Lichtenstein, P. Slovic, B. Fischhoff, M. Layman, and B. Combs, “Judged frequency of lethal events,” J. of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, Vol.4, pp. 551-578, 1978.
- [15] S. E. Spedden and P. B. Ryan, “Probabilistic connotations of carcinogen hazard classifications: Analysis of survey data for anchoring effects,” Risk Anal., Vol.12, pp. 535-541, 1992.
- [16] J. Costa-Font, C. Rudisill, and E. Mossialos, “Attitudes as an expression of knowledge and “political anchoring”: The case of nuclear power in the United Kingdom,” Risk Anal., Vol.28, pp. 1273-1288, 2008.
- [17] S. Oki and K. Nakayachi, “Paradoxical effects of the record-high tsunamis caused by the 2011 Tohoku earthquake on public judgments of danger,” Intl. J. of Disaster Risk Reduction, Vol.2, pp. 37-45, 2012.
- [18] T. D. Wilson, C. E. Houston, K. M. Etling, and N. Brekke, “A new look at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents,” J. of Experimental Psychology, Vol.125, pp. 387-402, 1996.
- [19] N. T. Brewer and G. B. Chapman, “The fragile basic anchoring effect,” J. of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol.15, pp. 65-77, 2002.
- [20] C. Critcher and T. Gilovich, “Incidental environmental anchors,” J. of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol.21, pp. 241-251. 2008.
- [21] D. Ariely, G. Loewenstein, and D. Prelec, “Coherent arbitrariness” : Stable demand curves without stable preferences,” The Quarterly J. of Economics, Vol.118, pp. 73-106, 2003.
- [22] K. F. E. Wong and J. Y. Y. Kwong, “Is 7300 m equal to 7.3 km? Same semantics but different anchoring effects,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol.82, pp. 314-333, 2000.
- [23] T. Mussweiler and F. Strack, “Comparing is believing: A selective accessibility model of judgmental anchoring,” European Review of Social Psychology, Vol.10, pp. 135-167, 1999.
- [24] F. Strack and T. Mussweiler, “Explaining the enigmatic anchoring effect: Mechanisms of selective accessibility,” J. of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.73, pp. 437-446, 1997.
- [25] T. Mussweiler and F. Strack, “Hypothesis-consistent testing and semantic priming in the anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility model,” J. of Experimental Social Psychology, Vol.35, pp. 136-164, 1999.
- [26] D. M. Oppenheimer, R. A. LeBoeuf, and N. T. Brewer, “Anchors aweigh: A demonstration of cross-modality anchoring and magnitude priming,” Cognition, Vol.106, pp. 13-26, 2008.
- [27] D. Sleeth-Keppler, “Taking the high (or low) road: A quantifier priming perspective on basic anchoring effects,” J. of Social Psychology, Vol.153, pp. 424-447, 2013.
- [28] National Research Council, “Improving risk communication,” National Academy Press, 1989.
- [29] Ministry of Economy, Trade and Industry (METI), “An proposal for the voluntary and continuous improvement of nuclear safety,” 2014, http://www.meti.go.jp/committee/sougouenergy/denryokutextunderscore gas/ genshiryoku/anzentextunderscore wg/pdf/report02textunderscore 01.pdf [accessed April. 19, 2015]
- [30] A. Chernev, “Semantic anchoring in sequential evaluations of vices and virtues,” J. of Consumer Research, Vol.37, pp. 761-774, 2011.
- [31] S. Epstein, “Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious,” American Psychologist, Vol.49, pp. 709-724, 1994.
- [32] S. A. Sloman, “The empirical case for two systems of reasoning,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol.119, pp. 3-22, 1996.
- [33] K. E. Stanovich and R. F. West, “Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?” In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman (Eds.), “Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment,” Cambridge University Press, pp. 421-444, 2002.
- [34] ISO, ISO31000:2009 “Risk management – Principles and guidelines,” 2009.
- [35] E. Watamura, T. Wakebe, and M. Saeki, “Experimental verification of the anchoring effect of a punishment reference histogram,” Japanese Society of Social Psychology, Vol.30, pp. 11-20, 2014.
- [36] R. Kanda, “Risk Communication in the field of radiation,” J. of Disaster Research, Vol.9, No.sp, pp. 608-618, 2014.
- [37] K. Nakayachi, “Toward mitigating actions: Risk communication regarding natural disaster,” J. of Disaster Research, Vol.9, No.sp, pp. 638-643, 2014.
- [38] P. Bubeck, W. J. W. Botzen, and J. C. J. H. Aerts, “A review of risk perceptions and other factors that influence flood mitigation behavior,” Risk Anal., Vol.32, pp. 1481-1495, 2012.
- [39] G. Wachinger, O. Renn, C. Begg, and C. Kuhlicke, “The risk perception paradox -implications for governance and communication of natural hazards,” Risk Anal., Vol.33, pp. 1049-1065, 2013.
- [40] N. D. Weinstein, “Optimistic biases about personal risks,” Science, Vol.24, pp. 1232-1233, 1989.
This article is published under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internationa License.