Research Paper:
Selection Prediction Models Considering Context Effects
Wenhao Zhang and Takashi Hasuike

Department of Industrial and Management Systems Engineering, Graduate School of Creative Science and Engineering, Waseda University
3-4-1 Okubo, Shinjuku-ku, Tokyo 169-8555, Japan
This study proposes a method for processing unordered datasets using deep learning techniques and introduces a model capable of simultaneously predicting three context effects and selections. “Context effects” refer to dramatic changes in the selection situation caused by the introduction of new options into a set of choices. Five experiments were conducted using different product-selection data. Each experiment focused on predicting context effects, selections, predictions under different context effects, predictions for different types of products, and predictions that considered context effects. The results demonstrated that the proposed model is suitable for classification prediction tasks in complex situations. The more complex the model and the larger the amount of data, the better the results. This study extends the application of neural networks to multi-attribute decision-making problems and contributes to the selection of decision-making models. It also improves the prediction accuracy and analyzes the impact of context effects on choices.

Compromise effect of moving the position of the selection set
- [1] D. He, “A behavioral analysis of the transformation from rational choice to irrational choice,” Economic Research J., Vol.2005, No.8, pp. 73-83, 2005 (in Chinese).
- [2] I. Simonson, “Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects,” J. of Consumer Research, Vol.16, No.2, pp. 158-174, 1989. https://doi.org/10.1086/209205
- [3] A. Tversky and I. Simonson, “Context-dependent preferences,” Management Science, Vol.39, No.10, pp. 1179-1189, 1993. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.39.10.1179
- [4] J. Devlin, M.-W. Chang, K. Lee, and K. Toutanova, “BERT: Pre-training of deep bidirectional transformers for language understanding,” Proc. of the 2019 Conf. on the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies, Vol.1, pp. 4171-4186, 2019. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/N19-1423
- [5] H.-T. Cheng et al., “Wide & deep learning for recommender systems,” arXiv:1606.07792, 2016. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1606.07792
- [6] A. Mottini and R. Acuna-Agost, “Deep choice model using pointer networks for airline itinerary prediction,” Proc. of the 23rd ACM SIGKDD Int. Conf. on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining, pp. 1575-1583, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1145/3097983.3098005
- [7] R. Dhar and R. Glazer, “Similarity in context: Cognitive representation and violation of preference and perceptual invariance in consumer choice,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol.67, No.3, pp. 280-293, 1996. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0080
- [8] J. Huber, J. W. Payne, and C. Puto, “Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis,” J. of Consumer Research, Vol.9, No.1, pp. 90-98, 1982. https://doi.org/10.1086/208899
- [9] J. R. Busemeyer, R. Barkan, S. Mehta, and A. Chaturvedi, “Context effects and models of preferential choice: Implications for consumer behavior,” Marketing Theory, Vol.7, No.1, pp. 39-58, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470593107073844
- [10] C.-C. Chang, S.-C. Chuang, Y.-H. Cheng, and T.-Y. Huang, “The compromise effect in choosing for others,” J. of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol.25, No.2, pp. 109-122, 2012. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.720
- [11] S.-C. Chuang, Y.-H. Cheng, C.-J. Chang, and Y.-T. Chiang, “The impact of self-confidence on the compromise effect,” Int. J. of Psychology, Vol.48, No.4, pp. 660-675, 2013. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.666553
- [12] M. Mourali, U. Böckenholt, and M. Laroche, “Compromise and attraction effects under prevention and promotion motivations,” J. of Consumer Research, Vol.34, No.2, pp. 234-247, 2007. https://doi.org/10.1086/519151
- [13] R. Dhar, S. M. Nowlis, and S. J. Sherman, “Trying hard or hardly trying: An analysis of context effects in choice,” J. of Consumer Psychology, Vol.9, No.4, pp. 189-200, 2000. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP0904_1
- [14] C.-F. Lee, S.-C. Chuang, C.-K. Chiu, and K.-H. Lan, “The influence of task difficulty on context effect—Compromise and attraction effects,” Current Psychology, Vol.36, No.3, pp. 392-409, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9428-0
- [15] I.-J. Park, J. Kim, J. Jhang, S. S. Kim, and V. Zhao, “How more options decrease the compromise effect: Investigating boundary conditions for the compromise effect in travel decisions,” J. of Travel Research, Vol.61, No.7, pp. 1542-1558, 2022. https://doi.org/10.1177/00472875211036193
- [16] R. Kivetz, O. Netzer, and V. Srinivasan, “Alternative models for capturing the compromise effect,” J. of Marketing Research, Vol.41, No.3, pp. 237-257, 2004. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.41.3.237.35990
- [17] R. M. Roe, J. R. Busemeyer, and J. T. Townsend, “Multialternative decision field theory: A dynamic connectionist model of decision making,” Psychological Review, Vol.108, No.2, pp. 370-392, 2001. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.370
- [18] J. R. Busemeyer and J. T. Townsend, “Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment,” Psychological Review, Vol.100, No.3, pp. 432-459, 1993. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.432
- [19] A. Tversky, “Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice,” Psychological Review, Vol.79, No.4, pp. 281-299, 1972. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032955
- [20] A. Tversky and I. Simonson, “Context-dependent preferences,” Management Science, Vol.39, No.10, pp. 1179-1189, 1993. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.39.10.1179
- [21] J. R. Busemeyer, R. K. Jessup, J. G. Johnson, and J. T. Townsend, “Building bridges between neural models and complex decision making behaviour,” Neural Networks, Vol.19, No.8, pp. 1047-1058, 2006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2006.05.043
- [22] A. Tversky and D. Kahneman, “Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty,” J. of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol.5, No.4, pp. 297-323, 1992. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574
- [23] A. Vaswani et al., “Attention is all you need,” arXiv:1706.03762, 2017. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1706.03762
- [24] S. Koatani, “Discrete choice model and predicting preference reversals via permutation equivalent neural networks,” Master’s thesis, Waseda University, 2019 (in Japanese).
- [25] M. Zaheer et al., “Deep sets,” Proc. of the 31st Int. Conf. on Neural Information Processing Systems, pp. 3394-3404, 2017.
- [26] J. Lee et al., “Set Transformer: A framework for attention-based permutation-invariant neural networks,” arXiv:1810.00825, 2019. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1810.00825
- [27] N. A. J. Berkowitsch, B. Scheibehenne, and J. Rieskamp, “Rigorously testing multialternative decision field theory against random utility models,” J. of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol.143, No.3, pp. 1331-1348, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035159
- [28] A. Puška, Ž. Stević, and D. Pamučar, “Evaluation and selection of healthcare waste incinerators using extended sustainability criteria and multi-criteria analysis methods,” Environment Development and Sustainability, Vol.24, No.9, pp. 11195-11225, 2022. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10668-021-01902-2
This article is published under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internationa License.