Paper:
Haptic Length Display Based on Cutaneous-Proprioceptive Integration
Kazunori Terada*, Akinori Kumazaki*, Daisuke Miyata**,
and Akira Ito*
*Gifu University, 1-1 Yanagido, Gifu 501-1193, Japan
**FUJI SOFT ABC INCORPORATED, 1-1 Sakuragi-cho, Naka-ku, Yokohama-shi, Kanagawa 231-8008, Japan
- [1] D. F. Collins, K. M. Refshauge, G. Todd, and S. C. Gandevia, “Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee,” Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol.94, pp. 1699-1706, 2005.
- [2] R. H. Cormack, “Haptic illusion: apparent elongation of a disk rotated between the fingers,” Science, Vol.179, No.73, pp. 590-592, 1973.
- [3] D. A. Corsini, and H. L. Pick Jr., “The effect of texture on tactually perceived length,” Perception & Psychophysics, Vol.5, No.6, pp. 352-356, 1969.
- [4] M. Degenaar, and G.-J. Lokhorst, “Molyneux’s problem,” In E. N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Fall, 2005.
- [5] J. J. DiCarlo, and K. O. Johnson, “Velocity invariance of receptive field structure in somatosensory cortical area 3b of the alert monkey,” The Journal of Neuroscience, Vol.19, No.1, pp. 401-419, 1999.
- [6] H. Faineteau, E. Gentaz, and P. Viviani, “The kinaesthetic perception of euclidean distance: a study of the detour effect,” Experimental Brain Research, Vol.152, No.2, pp. 166-172, 2003.
- [7] J. J. Gibson, “Observations on active touch,” Psychological Review, Vol.69, No.6, pp. 477-491, 1962.
- [8] H. Helson, “The tau effect.an example of psychological relativity,” Science, Vol.71, No.1847, pp. 536-537, 1930.
- [9] A. Hohmuth, W. D. Phillips, and H. VanRomer, “A discrepancy between two modes of haptic length perception,” The Journal of Psychology, Vol.92, pp. 79-87, 1976.
- [10] M. Hollins, and A. K. Goble, “Perception of the length of voluntary movements,” Somatosensory Research, Vol.5, No.4, pp. 335-348, 1988.
- [11] M. Hollins, and S. R. Risner, “Evidence for the duplex theory of tactile texture perception,” Perception & Psychophysics, Vol.62, No.4, pp. 695-705, 2000.
- [12] K. O. Johnson, and J. R. Phillips, “A rotating drum stimulator for scanning embossed patterns and textures across the skin,” Journal of Neuroscience Methods, Vol.22, No.3, pp. 221-231, 1988.
- [13] J. R. Pruett Jr., R. J. Sinclair, and H. Burton, “Response patterns in second somatosensory cortex (SII) of awake monkeys to passively applied tactile gratings,” The Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol.84, No.2, pp. 780-797, 2000.
- [14] D. Kats, “The world of touch,” Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1989.
- [15] S. J. Lederman, R. L. Klatzky, and P. O. Barber, “Spatial and movementbased heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol.114, No.1, pp. 33-49, 1985.
- [16] E. M. Meftah, L. Belingard, and C. E. Chapman, “Relative effects of the spatial and temporal characteristics of scanned surfaces on human perception of tactile roughness using passive touch,” Experimental Brain Research, Vol.132, No.3, pp. 351- 361, 2000.
- [17] V. Smetacek, and F. Mechsner, “Making sense,” Nature, Vol.432, No.21, 2004.
- [18] R. J. van Beers, D. M. Wolpert, and P. Haggard, “When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation,” Current Biology, Vol.12, No.10, pp. 843-847, 2002.
- [19] S. Wapner, J. Weinberg, J. A. Glick, and G. Rand, “Effect of speed of movement on tactualkinesthetic perception of extent,” American Journal of Psychology, Vol.80, No.4, pp. 608-613, 1967.
- [20] B. L. Whitsel, O. Franzen, D. A. Dreyer, M. Hollins, M. Young, G. K. Essick, and C. Wong, “Dependence of subjective traverse length on velocity of moving tactile stimuli,” Somatosensory Research, Vol.3, No.3, pp. 185-196, 1986.
This article is published under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internationa License.
Copyright© 2006 by Fuji Technology Press Ltd. and Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers. All right reserved.